Skip to main navigation menu Skip to main content Skip to site footer

ARTIGOS

Vol. 12 No. 1 (2024): Percepta - Revista de Cognição Musical

Musical and Rhythmic Exposure in Baby Development: a bibliographic research

DOI
https://doi.org/10.34018/2318-891X.12(1)1-18
Submitted
October 28, 2024
Published
2025-02-12

Abstract

This study investigated the influence of exposure to musical and rhythmic stimuli on the cognitive, motor, emotional, and social development of infants. The theoretical framework was based on Piaget's theories of cognitive development and Gordon's theories of musical learning. The methodology employed a qualitative approach, bibliographic research, and data collection via the internet, analyzing seven studies published between 2003 and 2023. Results indicate positive effects of musical and rhythmic stimulation on neurobehavioral, cerebral, motor, and cognitive development, as well as emotional and analgesic responses. It was observed that music promotes brain growth, improves emotional regulation, enhances motor skills, and offers analgesic effects in medical procedures, especially in premature infants. Exposure to musical and rhythmic stimuli and the integration of musical and rhythmic experiences from the first months of life are fundamental in child development.

References

  1. Amirkhanzadeh Barandouzi, Z., Keshavarz, M., Ashayeri, H., & Montazeri, A. (2012). Effect of lullaby music on pain in preterm infants during venipuncture. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 97(Suppl 2), A538. https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2012-302724.1903
  2. Bardin, L. (2011). Análise de conteúdo. São Paulo: Edições 70.
  3. Cirelli, L. K., Einarson, K. M., & Trainor, L. J. (2014). Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Developmental Science, 17(6), 1003-1011.
  4. Creswell, J. W. (2014). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methods approaches (4th ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
  5. Flick, U. (2009). An introduction to qualitative research (4th ed.). London: Sage Publications.
  6. Gil, A. C. (2008). Métodos e técnicas de pesquisa social (6ª ed.). São Paulo: Atlas.
  7. Gordon, E. E. (2003). A music learning theory for newborn and young children. GIA Publications.
  8. Gordon, E. E. (1997). Learning sequences in music: Skill, content, and patterns. GIA Publications.
  9. Hallam, S. (2010). The power of music: Its impact on the intellectual, social and personal development of children and young people. International Journal of Music Education, 28(3), 269-289.
  10. Haslbeck, F. B., Bucher, H.-U., Bassler, D., & Hagmann, C. (2017). Creative music therapy to promote brain structure, function, and neurobehavioral outcomes in preterm infants: a randomized controlled pilot trial protocol. Pilot and Feasibility Studies, 3, 36. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40814-017-0180-5
  11. Ilari, B. (2016). Music in the early years: Pathways into the social world. Research Studies in Music Education, 38(1), 23-39.
  12. Inamdar, K., Khurana, S., & Dusing, S. C. (2023). Effect of contingency paradigm–based interventions on developmental outcomes in young infants: A systematic review. Pediatric Physical Therapy, 35(2), 146-161. https://doi.org/10.1097/PEP.0000000000000924
  13. Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Joint drumming: Social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102(3), 299-314.
  14. Malloch, S., & Trevarthen, C. (Eds.). (2009). Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship. Oxford University Press.
  15. Piaget, J. (1936). La naissance de l'intelligence chez l'enfant. Delachaux et Niestlé.
  16. Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. International Universities Press.
  17. Provasi, J., Blanc, L., & Carchon, I. (2021). The importance of rhythmic stimulation for preterm infants in the NICU. Children, 8(8), 660. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8080660
  18. Provasi, J., & Bobin-Bègue, A. (2003). Spontaneous motor tempo and rhythmical synchronisation in 2½- and 4-year-old children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(3), 220-231.
  19. Schmidt, L. A., Trainor, L. J., & Santesso, D. L. (2003). Development of frontal electroencephalogram (EEG) and heart rate (ECG) responses to affective musical stimuli during the first 12 months of post-natal life. Brain and Cognition, 52(1), 27-32. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00007-1
  20. Sievers, B., Polansky, L., Casey, M., & Wheatley, T. (2013). Music and movement share a dynamic structure that supports universal expressions of emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(1), 70-75. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1209023110
  21. Siu, T.-S. C., & Cheung, H. (2017). Infants' sensitivity to emotion in music and emotion-action understanding. PLOS ONE, 12(2), e0171023. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171023
  22. Trainor, L. J. (2018). The origins of music in auditory scene analysis and the roles of evolution and culture in musical creation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1751), 20170071.
  23. Trehub, S. E. (2003). The developmental origins of musicality. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 669-673.
  24. Zentner, M., & Eerola, T. (2010). Rhythmic engagement with music in infancy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(13), 5768-5773.

Similar Articles

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 > >> 

You may also start an advanced similarity search for this article.